The recent surge of interest into the role of diet in the development of chronic lifestyle disease has brought scrutiny onto the composition of diets in in Western industrialised countries, with a particular emphasis on extrinsic, added sugars.
While extrinsic sugars cannot be blamed in isolation, they are a contributing factor in the development of lifestyle- related disease insofar as they drive an energy surplus, together with other energy-dense dietary constituents (1). While the crux of the issue is increased overall energy consumption in the diet, the evidence suggests that under free-living conditions the primary issue with added sugars is that they are consumed in the diet without any compensatory reduction in energy intake (2). Thus, increasing the proportion of sugar in the diet under ad libitum conditions drives increasing adiposity where calories are not in turn reduced from other sources (2).
As a strategy to reduce calorie intake in the population, interest has turned to the use of non- nutritive sweeteners [NNS], which encompasses both synthetic artificial sweeteners [AS] and non-caloric sweeteners of natural origin (3). However, despite extensive toxicology studies and both pre and post-market research, significant concerns continue to be raised in relation to the safety and efficacy of artificial sweeteners (4). Among the more common concerns expressed relate to potential carcinogenic effects, causing weight gain, stimulating blood glucose, activating brain reward circuitry and stimulating hunger or appetite (5).
In this article we’ll focus on artificial sweeteners only, specifically looking at the main AS currently in use: acesulfame-K, aspartame, saccharin, and sucralose (5). This group of compounds are frequently considered under the umbrella term ‘artificial sweetener’, as they are all defined by having a greater sweetness intensity than sugar, allowing low levels to be added to foods and drinks as a caloric substitute (6). However, it should be noted that each compound is structurally unique, meaning the individual compounds all vary in sweetness potency, duration of sweetness, aftertaste, and mouth feel (6). They also have different pharmacokinetic profiles which warrants individual consideration of their health effects, not collectively under the umbrella term ‘AS’ (6). This article will refer specifically to each AS where appropriate.
Regulation of Artificial Sweeteners and Safety Thresholds
An important first step in dispelling some myths in relation to AS is understanding the regulatory processes through which the Acceptable Daily Intake [ADI] for any food additive is set. Compounds are not recklessly unleashed into the food environment for human consumption without regulatory oversight and safety evaluations. The AS listed above are approved for use in the European Union [EU] through the European Food Standards Agency [EFSA] and in the U.S. by the Food and Drug Administration [FDA], through a process involving submission of both scientific safety evaluation and technical data (6; 8). The technical data includes the chemical composition of the compound, its source and
manufacturing methods, its stability across a range of food matrices, and sensory properties (6; 8).
The safety data contained in the submission process must include the full range of studies on safety, including the anticipated daily intake in the population from all dietary sources, within different ages groups (6; 8). This safety data submitted is derived from animal toxicology studies that have specific criteria for the design and type of the trials required based on a system of “Concern Levels”. Due to their potential high exposure in the population, and toxicity potential, AS are considered a “high concern” level (6; 8). The effect of this is concern level is that toxicology studies must be conducted in animals with a similar pharmacokinetic profile to humans, and must assess both absolute toxicity thresholds and also sub-chronic long-term toxicity for potential effects on reproduction, development, carcinogenicity, genotoxicity, and immunotoxicity (6).
Based on the lowest threshold of any toxicity observed in safety studies animal toxicology studies, the “No Observable Adverse Effect Level” [NOAEL] is established (7). The ADI is then established by dividing the NOAEL by an “uncertainty factor” of 100 (7). The technical data is used to assess potential health impacts from exposure to the compound in the food supply, by combining data on anticipated intake by reference to the concentrations of the compound in food and beverages together with the quantity of those foods/drinks typically consumed (7). The requirement for this calculation is to combine the maximum permitted level of the compound in foods together with the maximum level of consumption of
food/drink (7). Like the establishment of the ADI, this is a highly conservative assessment, particularly for children and the elderly.
Understanding the Difference Between the ADI, the NOAEL and Consumption Levels
Let’s put this information into some context. The NOAEL for aspartame was set at 4g per kilogram of bodyweight per day [4g/kg/bw/d] based on long-term animal toxicology studies (7; 8). The ADI was then set at 40mg/kg in the E.U., and 50mg/kg in the U.S., which is the equivalent of a 60kg adult consuming 4L of artificially sweetened beverages containing the maximum level of aspartame permitted under regulation (7). However, no single food or beverage contains the maximum permitted level of any AS. In the case of aspartame, average estimated intake is 1/10th the ADI – 4mg/kg (9;10). The highest population subsets of consumers still consume under 30% of the ADI, with the 90th percentile consuming an average of 10mg/kg/d (9; 10). Recall that we are assessing consumption by reference to the ADI, not the NOAEL. Even assuming an adult did consume the full ADI for aspartame, that dose would still be 1/100 th of the NOAEL, which is the lowest dose without any observed negative effect in long-term animal toxicology studies (7).
For the AS saccharin, the ADI is 5mg/kg/bw/d: to consume the ADI you would have to drink 800 330ml cans of diet soda sweetened with saccharin (11). For acesulfame-K, the ADI is 15mg/kg, and average consumption is estimated at 20% of the ADI over the course of a lifetime (11). Sucralose, known under the brand ‘Splenda’ and one of the most widely used
AS, has an ADI of 5mg/kg, but there is no bioaccumulation of sucralose (9).
There is an important point to bear in mind in light of the significant gap between toxicity thresholds and consumption: just because something has toxicity potential doesn’t mean it is. There is a lower toxicity threshold for vitamin A, copper, or selenium than artificial sweeteners. Many people are making a false distinction based on the well-known ‘appeal to
nature’ fallacy: that because something is “artificial” it must be bad for health, while anything “natural” is therefore good. Ironically, you’d have more chance of suffering selenium toxicity from consuming too many Brazil nuts than reaching toxicity thresholds for AS.
It is crucial to consider the ADI and NOAEL when discussing the purported adverse effects of AS. For example, a recent study looking at neurotoxic effects of aspartame used doses of 500mg/kg and 1,000mg/kg in rats (12), which are 2,400% greater than the ADI and thus of no relevance to either established safety thresholds or to population levels of consumption. Nonetheless there remain concerns over the safety of AS for human consumption, so due diligence is warranted in assessing human health effects.
Artificial Sweeteners & Carcinogenicity
Saccharin, aspartame, and acesulfame-K have generated most concerns over potential carcinogenicity. In relation to saccharin, concerns arose from early animal toxicology studies in the 1970’s in which bladder cancer developed in rats administered high doses. However, further research found that the carcinogenic mechanisms identified in rodents were not applicable to humans, and subsequent studies have found no associations between saccharin consumption and cancer in humans (11; 13).
Research on aspartame is generally controversial due to the strong associations between study outcomes and funding source. Analysis of this issue found that industry funded studies all attest to safety, while 92% of independently funded studies report adverse health effects, particularly in relation to body weight, diabetes/glucose regulation [more on this below] (14). These inconsistencies mandated the need for an unbiased evaluation of empirical evidence in
relation to aspartame and carcinogenesis. A 2015 meta-analysis of carcinogenic bioassay animal studies concluded there was no significant relationship between various experimental doses of aspartame and occurrence of cancerous tumours (14). This is consistent with observational epidemiology in humans (7).
It is important to note that much of the controversy regarding aspartame stems from 3 studies from the same research group in Europe, all of which purported to show carcinogenicity in rats and mice (15; 16; 17). The 2015 meta-analysis included these studies, lending weight to its conclusions (14). The European Food Standards Agency have rejected the findings of the latest study (17) on the basis that the tumours observed in mice were not mechanistically relevant to humans (18). More concerningly, the researchers misdiagnosed hyperplasia as malignant tumours and violated OECD testing protocols by administering aspartame during fetal development (7; 18). Unfortunately, these studies generate unscientific assertions that run contrary to the fact that there is no strong evidence that aspartame is carcinogenic (19;
20). This should be interpreted in the context of recent re-analyses of aspartame by both the EFSA and the FDA which both concluded that the current ADI is appropriate having regard to potential risk and current levels of consumption (18; 5).
Acesulfame-K has also been controversial due to its approval by the FDA prior to the standardisation of animal carcinogenic bioassays, leading to criticism that the studies assessing carcinogenicity are inadequate (21). As a “second generation” AS approved in the late 1980’s, there is a lack of human observational epidemiology in relation to acesulfame-K and cancer (19). Research by the National Toxicology Program in the United States failed to find any carcinogenic effect in rodent models (22). However, these toxicology studies failed to comply with the 24-month duration recommended by the FDA in its toxicology study design guidelines (23). Controversy over the use of acesulfame-K has continued in the U.S. as the NTP has rejected submissions to reevaluate its safety (23). The EU, on the other hand, has ongoing toxicology monitoring programs which continually review food additives, including AS. Acesulfame-K was reevaluated in 2000 by the EU Scientific Committee on Food [SCF], including consideration of the claims that the original toxicology studies were inadequate, and concluded concerns over carcinogenicity were not substantiated (24). The
SCF was subsequently succeeded by the EFSA, and in 2009 the EU mandated that all AS in the food supply had to be reevaluated by the EFSA, including updated technical and toxicology (8). Acesulfame-K was concluded to be safe, including an extension of its permitted to children up to 3-years of age (25). Leaving aside the controversy in the US,
which in reality is a regulatory issue, the ongoing scientific review by independent regulatory bodies of the EU show acesulfame-K is not a safety concern at habitual levels of consumption, which are a fraction of the established ADI (26; 24).
Sucralose is one of the most comprehensively researched AS with a strong safety profile, including for consumption during pregnancy and in children (11). A recent review of toxicology research found no evidence of carcinogenicity in long-term animal models or in human studies (27). The research on sucralose highlights an important factor to consider:
toxicology programs are designed to induce a toxic response, and there is substantial regulatory oversight in ensuring rigorous standardisation of the toxicology models (27). To date, numerous international agencies and scientists working in the field continue to support the safety of AS for human consumption (5).
There is often criticism levelled at the regulatory framework in which AS are approved for us, posited as: “who regulates the regulators”. It is important to point out that the EU has banned over 1,300 compounds from use in personal care and cosmetic products based on even preliminary evidence of toxicology (28). The criticism fails to stand up to an unbiased,
objective assessment of the regulatory framework in which the safety of AS are analysed. Nonetheless, using animal bioassays to inform health risks of long-term human consumption will always carry a margin of uncertainty. It is thus difficult from a scientific standpoint to consider the carcinogenic potential of AS a closed case. However, within current toxicology monitoring programs and at average consumption levels in the population, the evidence does
not currently support a carcinogenic effect of authorised AS in humans.
Artificial Sweeteners & Body Weight
Several observational human studies have observed a relationship between high or regular diet soda consumption and increased adiposity and cardiometabolic risk (29). Teasing this apart, it is clear that the increase in cardiometabolic risk factors is a consequence of increasing adiposity (29). The relevant question is the relationship between non-calorically sweetened beverage consumption and weight gain. Closer scrutiny of the observational research reveals that the relationship is in fact the other way around: higher adiposity is associated with high levels of AS intake (3). This is known as “reverse causality”, i.e. those with high AS consumption are more likely to have a poor, high calorie diet and have higher adiposity (3).
There is a clear distinction between observational research and controlled trials, and there are multiple randomised controlled trials looking at this issue. A recent meta-analysis of 15 RCT’s found that use of AS was clearly associated with lower body weight, BMI and waist circumference (30). The included RCT’s comprised a large sample size, including 4 studies in children, with no study finding that AS led to negative weight outcomes (30). This is consistent with the expected purpose of AS substituting for sugar and/or calories in beverages and food products, which is to reduce total energy intake and contribute to weight loss (3). This is consistently observed in RCT’s (30).
It should be stressed that this effect assumes that compensations are not made in energy intake by substituting AS and increasing calories from other sources: the reverse causality observed in observational studies. This is a behavioural issue and cannot be specifically attributed to AS. Controlled data supports basic energy balance fundamentals, and when AS products substitute for calorie-containing products, individuals lose weight (3; 30). However, certain research has suggested other mechanisms by which AS may contribute to weight gain, including stimulating blood glucose and “tricking” the brain through activation of brain-reward circuitry which responds to sugar/sweet taste, in turn stimulating appetite (31; 32). These concerns warrant closer scrutiny.
Artificial Sweeteners & Blood Glucose Regulation
The hypothesis is that the intense sweet perception of AS alters glycemic and insulin responses, and sweet-taste receptors in the gut are activated in response to AS, increasing glucose uptake. In relation to glycaemia and insulin, a trial in morbidly obese subjects but with normal insulin sensitivity [assessed by HOMA-IR] found greater peak plasma glucose levels and insulin response to an oral glucose tolerance test [OGTT] given 10-mins after a pre-load with 48mg of sucralose (33). This study has challenged previous research on sucralose, which was considered to have no impact on carbohydrate metabolism from multiple studies looking at glycemic responses (34).
It is difficult to reconcile the current literature on sucralose in relation to glycaemic or insulin responses. In the E.U. SCF review of sucralose safety, one 6-month study in Type-2 diabetics found a consistent increase above baseline in HbA1c [a marker of long-term glycemic control] in subjects given 667mg sucralose daily, however, this occurred in the absence of
any impacts on insulin or blood glucose levels (35). The SCF review ultimately concluded that the doses administered were significantly greater than consumption levels, such that any actual effect at population levels of consumption would be so small as to be clinically insignificant (35). In this context, note that in the trial which found greater peak plasma glucose and insulin responses to an OGTT, the responses still remained within normal range
for OGTT and amounted to nominal differences between sucralose group and controls (36). The inconsistencies in the research are evident in trials on sucralose: one study found higher blood glucose levels, one study reported lower blood glucose levels, and nine studies have found no effect (36).
A recent systematic review of 28 trials of aspartame, sucralose, saccharin, and acesulfame-K confirmed these inconsistencies (36). Certain trials have found effects of AS on glucose metabolism, most have not found any interaction, and the significant differences in the studies between subjects, the AS used, the placebo, and outcome variables, limits
comparisons (36). Of these issues, perhaps the most important is the comparison of AS to placebo, often water, where arguably the appropriate trial design is a comparison with a caloric sweetener (3). This is because, if we assume AS do stimulate glucose uptake, then this would occur in the context of low concentrations of glucose in the digestive tract, while a caloric sweetener like sucrose would result in a greater amount of glucose absorbed (37). This may explain why in trials using OGTT the observed differences in absorption between an AS beverage and water are largely nominal (38).
The second arm of the hypothesis is that AS activate glucose transporters in the gut, with one proposed mechanism being activation of glucagon-like peptide-1 [GLP-1]. This research is also equivocal, evident in a systematic review including 11 studies which assessed GLP-1: aspartame has been shown to lower GLP-1, while sucralose and acesulfame-K found to increase GLP-1 concentrations (36). Yes again, this is not consistent. In a trial administering
72mg aspartame and 24mg sucralose before a 75g OGTT, no effect of either AS on GLP-1, insulin or glucose in Type-2 diabetics was found (39). For more confusion, the healthy control subjects had a significantly higher GLP-1 area-under-the-curve from sucralose, but not aspartame (39).
The real issue here is arguably trial design, an example of which is evident in two trials. The first used diet soda containing 46mg sucralose plus 26mg acesulfame-K as the intervention in healthy humans, and found GLP-1 increased in response to OGTT compared to water (40). However, the trial failed to control for other compounds in the diet soda – citric acid, phosphoric acid, potassium benzoate, potassium citrate, natural colourings and flavourings – which may have influenced the results (40). Consequently, a subsequent trial administered 46mg sucralose plus 26mg acesulfame-K, or 52mg sucralose, or 200mg acesulfame-K alone, to healthy subjects and found no effect of either treatment on GLP-1, blood glucose, or insulin concentrations (41). This is consistent with another trial showing no effect of sucralose administered through intraduodenal glucose feeding on GLP-1 or glucose uptake (42).
Taking this research as a whole, a couple of trials show some effect of AS on GLP-1 and glycemic response (39; 40), while other studies (41; 42) and a systematic review of 11 studies measuring GLP-1 (37) suggest the majority of human studies have found no effect of AS on intestinal sweet-taste receptors and glucose uptake. The biological basis for the limited
studies finding such effect in humans remains unexplained, and the clinical significance of the findings remains questionable (35; 37). On the basis of in vitro [cell culture] and animal model studies, some authors and commentators have reached outside the data to make speculative assumptions on the consequences of sweet-taste receptor activation on glucose regulation (37). While potential mechanisms have been elucidated in vitro and in animal models, with limited supporting human data, the nominal effects may be of no clinical relevance. The overall body of evidence for a direct effect of AS on glycemic control is limited (3). It would be an overreach from the current evidence to say that AS influence blood glucose, insulin, or GLP-1 in humans: equally, it would be an overreach to say that AS are biologically inert.
Artificial Sweeteners and Activation of Brain Reward Circuits & Appetite
The hypothesis proposed here is that the intense sweetness of AS activates responses in brain regions that control energy balance. The evidence suggests, however, that for hypothalamic responses to occur there must be a coupling of sweet taste with actual energy content (11). Interestingly, this has been shown in sports nutrition research investigating the effects of carbohydrate mouth rinsing on performance, which found that the presence of glucose was necessary to elicit a brain response (43). The trial used functional magnetic resonance imaging [fMRI] to determine activation of brain-reward regions in response to either glucose, maltodextrin [a simple sugar] or saccharin intake (43). While both of the carbohydrate-containing solutions activated dopaminergic pathways that mediate reward-responses to food, these circuits which were unresponsive to saccharin (43). This is consistent with another trial comparing sucrose [table sugar] to sucralose [AS] which showed only sucrose led to brain activation responses (44). This research indicates that the presence of sweet taste alone is insufficient to activate brain regions with a role in appetite or food-reward behaviours, and requires the coupling of energy content to sweet taste as would be found in simple sugars (43;44)
Notwithstanding the studies using fMRI to determine brain responses, arguments remain that the disconnect between sweetness and energy content diminish control over energy intake and precipitate further energy intake (45). This is not supported in the literature. A systematic review and meta-analyses on the effect of AS consumption on energy intake found that in studies using pre-loads of AS beverages followed by ad libitum test meal, consumption of AS resulted in a reduction in overall energy, an effect consistent with longer-term RCT’s (45). This is corroborated by another systematic review which identified 7 studies investigating subjective appetite scores, none of which found any effect of AS consumption on subjective appetite (36). Cumulatively, the literature does not support any direct effect of AS on hunger and either subjective or objective appetite (46). That AS “trick the brain” into a response in the absence of carbohydrate is also unsupported (43; 44).
Artificial Sweeteners & The Microbiome
Research on the human gut microbiome has demonstrated that the composition of bacteria in the gut, which is strongly impacted by diet, influences host health (47). The primary driver of change in the microbiome is the presence or absence of dietary fibre, which undergo selective fermentation by bacteria species which specialise in the degradation of indigestible plant matter (48). Concerns over the impact of AS on the gut microbiome have been generated largely from in vitro studies, and there is limited quality human evidence (49). A recent study in mice found that saccharin, aspartame, and sucralose intake led to alterations in the gut microbiome that resulted in increased glucose intolerance (50). These results may not extrapolate to humans. For example, aspartame is comprised of the amino acids phenylalanine and aspartic acid, and is broken down into its component parts in the human small intestine: the individual component parts are absorbed, not aspartame itself (7). More particularly, the conclusions of the study were based on interpretation of data from fecal samples in rodents extrapolated to humans, and the findings should be interpreted with caution (49).
An issue with this emerging area of research is the lack of established models, and research designs have yet to be determined (49). There is evidence that sugar alcohols commonly used as AS may have “prebiotic” effects, increasing concentrations of beneficial bacterial populations in humans (51). Nonetheless, suggestive mechanisms for metabolic consequences from altered bacterial composition as a result of AS intake have been identified in animal models (52). The identified bacterial compositions correlate with alterations observed in the microbiome in humans with metabolic disease (52). While there can be no conclusion on cause vs. consequence yet, these effects warrant further investigation in well-controlled human studies with recognised models.
This area of research serves as an indictment of the hyperbole surrounding AS: those against AS use liberally cite limited and questionable animal model data as concrete evidence of a negative effect in humans, while others casually assume a benign effect. The reality is equivocal: at this moment in time, we cannot say definitively either way.
Conclusion
Can we definitively say that artificial sweeteners are benign?
Can we say concretely that there are no long-term consequences for human health from AS
consumption?
In relation to both questions, the answer – which many people don’t want to hear – is that we can never be 100% certain. Objectively and considering the totality of evidence, it does appear that much of the profoundly negative effects are exaggerated. Arguably at current habitual population levels of consumption, AS use would not appear to negatively impact on human health to any identifiable degree. Yet, having regard to grey areas in the literature and the reliance on animal models from toxicology to the microbiome, that cannot be stated with absolute authority.
Can we as healthcare professionals recommend artificially sweetened beverages and/or foods as a means to improve health? AS can certainly be an effective substitution for caloric-containing foods and beverages, provided those calories are not compensated for elsewhere in the diet. Insofar as weight loss may be required for clinically meaningful reductions in lifestyle disease risk, if the targeted use of AS use leads to a reduction in energy intake, that must be considered a positive intervention. However, I do think healthcare professionals should err on the side of caution and avoid recommending overly liberal use. On the other hand, fear-mongering over moderate AS consumption is counterproductive and, importantly for healthcare professionals, is not evidence-based advice. Eating more vegetables is more important than the odd Diet Coke.
In sum, artificial sweeteners can be included in the context of overall nutritional best practices. They should be a tool, not a crutch. In my opinion, that is a responsible position stand having regard to the overall literature.
References:
1. Khan, T. and Sievenpiper, J. (2016). Controversies about sugars: results from systematic reviews and meta-analyses on obesity, cardiometabolic disease and diabetes. European Journal of Nutrition, 55(S2), pp.25-43.
2. Te Morenga, L., Mallard, S. and Mann, J. (2012). Dietary sugars and body weight:
systematic review and meta-analyses of randomised controlled trials and cohort
studies. BMJ, 346(jan15 3), pp.e7492-e7492.
3. Gardner, C. (2014). Non-nutritive sweeteners. Current Opinion in Lipidology, 25(1), pp.80-84.
4. Martyn, D., Nugent, A., McNulty, B., O’Reilly, E., Tlustos, C., Walton, J., Flynn, A. and Gibney, M. (2016). Dietary intake of four artificial sweeteners by Irish pre-school children. Food Additives & Contaminants: Part A, pp.1-11.
5. Lohner, S., Toews, I. and Meerpohl, J. (2017). Health outcomes of non-nutritive sweeteners: analysis of the research landscape. Nutrition Journal, 16(1).
6. Roberts, A. (2016). The safety and regulatory process for low calorie sweeteners in the United States. Physiology & Behavior, 164, pp.439-444.
7. Marinovich, M., Galli, C., Bosetti, C., Gallus, S. and La Vecchia, C. (2013). Aspartame, low-calorie sweeteners and disease: Regulatory safety and epidemiological issues. Food and Chemical Toxicology, 60, pp.109-115
8. REGULATION (EC) No 1333/2008 OF THE EUROPEAN PARLIAMENT AND OF THE COUNCIL of 16 December 2008 on food additives.
9. Butchko, H. (1997). Safety of aspartame. The Lancet, 349(9058), p.1105.
10. Magnuson, B., Burdock, G., Doull, J., Kroes, R., Marsh, G., Pariza, M., Spencer, P., Waddell, W., Walker, R. and Williams, G. (2007). Aspartame: A Safety Evaluation Based on Current Use Levels, Regulations, and Toxicological and Epidemiological Studies. Critical Reviews in Toxicology, 37(8), pp.629-727.
11. Shankar, P., Ahuja, S. and Sriram, K. (2013). Non-nutritive sweeteners: Review and update. Nutrition, 29(11-12), pp.1293-1299.
12. Abhilash, M., Sauganth Paul, M., Varghese, M. and Nair, R. (2012). Long-term consumption of aspartame and brain antioxidant defense status. Drug and Chemical Toxicology, 36(2), pp.135-140.
13. Gallus, S., Scotti, L., Negri, E., Talamini, R., Franceschi, S., Montella, M., Giacosa, A., Dal Maso, L. and La Vecchia, C. (2006). Artificial sweeteners and cancer risk in a network of case-control studies. Annals of Oncology, 18(1), pp.40-44.
14. Mallikarjun, S. and Sieburth, R. (2013). Aspartame and Risk of Cancer: A Meta- analytic Review. Archives of Environmental & Occupational Health, 70(3), pp.133-141.
15. Soffritti, M., Belpoggi, F., Esposti, D., Lambertini, L., Tibaldi, E. and Rigano, A. (2005). First Experimental Demonstration of the Multipotential Carcinogenic Effects of Aspartame Administered in the Feed to Sprague-Dawley Rats. Environmental Health Perspectives, 114(3), pp.379-385.
16. Soffritti, M., Belpoggi, F., Tibaldi, E., Esposti, D. and Lauriola, M. (2007). Life-Span Exposure to Low Doses of Aspartame Beginning during Prenatal Life Increases Cancer Effects in Rats. Environmental Health Perspectives, 115(9), pp.1293-1297.
17. Soffritti, M., Belpoggi, F., Manservigi, M., Tibaldi, E., Lauriola, M., Falcioni, L. and Bua, L. (2010). Aspartame administered in feed, beginning prenatally through life span, induces cancers of the liver and lung in male Swiss mice. American Journal of Industrial Medicine, 53(12), pp.1197-1206.
18. Statement of EFSA on the scientific evaluation of two studies related to the safety of artificial sweeteners. (2011). EFSA Journal, 9(2), p.2089.
19. Weihrauch, M. and Diehl, V. (2004). Artificial sweeteners—do they bear a carcinogenic risk?. Annals of Oncology, 15(10), pp.1460-1465.
20. Bosetti, C., Gallus, S., Talamini, R., Montella, M., Franceschi, S., Negri, E. and La Vecchia, C. (2009). Artificial Sweeteners and the Risk of Gastric, Pancreatic, and Endometrial Cancers in Italy. Cancer Epidemiology Biomarkers & Prevention, 18(8), pp.2235-2238.
21. Woods, D. (1996). US scientists challenge approval of sweetener. BMJ, 313(7054), pp.386-386.
22. National Toxicology Program (2005). NTP toxicology studies of acesulfame potassium (CAS No. 55589-62-3) in genetically modified (FVB Tg.AC Hemizygous) mice and carcinogenicity studies of acesulfame potassium in genetically modified [B6.129-Trp53(tm1Brd) (N5) Haploinsufficient] mice (feed studies)mice. Natl Toxicol Program Genet Modif Model Rep., Oct(2), pp.1-113.
23. Karstadt, M. (2006). Testing Needed for Acesulfame Potassium, an Artificial Sweetener. Environmental Health Perspectives, 114(9), pp.A516-A516.
24. Mortensen, A. (2006). Sweeteners permitted in the European Union: safety aspects. Food & Nutrition Research, 50(3).
25. European Food Safety Authority (2016). Safety of the proposed extension of use of acesulfame K (E 950) in foods for special medical purposes in young children. EFSA Journal, 14(4), p.4437.
26. Lino, C., Costa, I., Pena, A., Ferreira, R. and Cardoso, S. (2008). Estimated intake of the sweeteners, acesulfame-K and aspartame, from soft drinks, soft drinks based on mineral waters and nectars for a group of Portuguese teenage students. Food Additives & Contaminants: Part A, 25(11), pp.1291-1296.
27. Berry, C., Brusick, D., Cohen, S., Hardisty, J., Grotz, V. and Williams, G. (2016). Sucralose Non-Carcinogenicity: A Review of the Scientific and Regulatory Rationale. Nutrition and Cancer, 68(8), pp.1247-1261.
28. Regulation (EC) No 1223/2009 of the European Parliament and of the Council of 30 November 2009 on cosmetic products.
29. Fowler, S. (2016). Low-calorie sweetener use and energy balance: Results from experimental studies in animals, and large-scale prospective studies in humans. Physiology & Behavior, 164, pp.517-523.
30. Miller, P. and Perez, V. (2014). Low-calorie sweeteners and body weight and composition: a meta-analysis of randomized controlled trials and prospective cohort studies. American Journal of Clinical Nutrition, 100(3), pp.765-777.
31. Peters, J. and Beck, J. (2016). Low Calorie Sweetener (LCS) use and energy balance. Physiology & Behavior, 164, pp.524-528.
32. Rogers, P. and Brunstrom, J. (2016). Appetite and energy balancing. Physiology & Behavior, 164, pp.465-471.
33. Pepino, M., Tiemann, C., Patterson, B., Wice, B. and Klein, S. (2013). Sucralose Affects Glycemic and Hormonal Responses to an Oral Glucose Load. Diabetes Care, 36(9), pp.2530-2535.
34. Brown, R. and Rother, K. (2012). Non-Nutritive Sweeteners and their Role in the Gastrointestinal Tract. The Journal of Clinical Endocrinology & Metabolism, 97(8), pp.2597-2605.
35. Scientific Committee on Food of the European Commission. Opinion of the Scientific Committee on Food on sucralose. SCF/CS/ADDS/EDUL/190 Final 12/9/2000.
36. Romo-Romo, A., Aguilar-Salinas, C., Brito-Córdova, G., Gómez Díaz, R., Valentín, D. and Almeda-Valdes, P. (2016). Effects of the Non-Nutritive Sweeteners on Glucose Metabolism and Appetite Regulating Hormones: Systematic Review of Observational Prospective Studies and Clinical Trials. PLoS One, 11(8), p.e0161264.
37. Renwick, A. and Molinary, S. (2010). Sweet-taste receptors, low-energy sweeteners, glucose absorption and insulin release. British Journal of Nutrition, 104(10), pp.1415- 1420.
38. Grotz, V. and Jokinen, J. (2014). Comment on Pepino et al. Sucralose Affects Glycemic and Hormonal Responses to an Oral Glucose Load. Diabetes Care 2013;36:2530–2535. Diabetes Care, 37(6), pp.e148-e148.
39. Temizkan, S., Deyneli, O., Yasar, M., Arpa, M., Gunes, M., Yazici, D., Sirikci, O., Haklar, G., Imeryuz, N. and Yavuz, D. (2014). Sucralose enhances GLP-1 release and lowers blood glucose in the presence of carbohydrate in healthy subjects but not in patients with type 2 diabetes. European Journal of Clinical Nutrition, 69(2), pp.162- 166.
40. Brown, R., Walter, M. and Rother, K. (2009). Ingestion of Diet Soda Before a Glucose Load Augments Glucagon-Like Peptide-1 Secretion. Diabetes Care, 32(12), pp.2184-2186.
41. Steinert, R., Frey, F., Töpfer, A., Drewe, J. and Beglinger, C. (2011). Effects of carbohydrate sugars and artificial sweeteners on appetite and the secretion of gastrointestinal satiety peptides. British Journal of Nutrition, 105(09), pp.1320-1328.
42. Ma, J., Chang, J., Checklin, H., Young, R., Jones, K., Horowitz, M. and Rayner, C. (2010). Effect of the artificial sweetener, sucralose, on small intestinal glucose absorption in healthy human subjects. British Journal of Nutrition, 104(06), pp.803-806.
43. Chambers, E., Bridge, M. and Jones, D. (2009). Carbohydrate sensing in the human mouth: effects on exercise performance and brain activity. The Journal of Physiology, 587(8), pp.1779-1794.
44. Frank, G., Oberndorfer, T., Simmons, A., Paulus, M., Fudge, J., Yang, T. and Kaye, W. (2008). Sucrose activates human taste pathways differently from artificial sweetener. NeuroImage, 39(4), pp.1559-1569.
45. Rogers, P., Hogenkamp, P., de Graaf, C., Higgs, S., Lluch, A., Ness, A., Penfold, C., Perry, R., Putz, P., Yeomans, M. and Mela, D. (2015). Does low-energy sweetener consumption affect energy intake and body weight? A systematic review, including meta-analyses, of the evidence from human and animal studies. International Journal of Obesity, 40(3), pp.381-394.
46. Raben, A. and Richelsen, B. (2012). Artificial sweeteners: a place in the field of functional foods? Focus on obesity and related metabolic disorders. Current Opinion in Clinical Nutrition and Metabolic Care, 15(6), pp.597-604.
47. Graf, D., Di Cagno, R., Fåk, F., Flint, H., Nyman, M., Saarela, M. and Watzl, B. (2015). Contribution of diet to the composition of the human gut microbiota. Microbial Ecology in Health & Disease, 26(0).
48. Walker, A., Ince, J., Duncan, S., Webster, L., Holtrop, G., Ze, X., Brown, D., Stares, M., Scott, P., Bergerat, A., Louis, P., McIntosh, F., Johnstone, A., Lobley, G., Parkhill, J. and Flint, H. (2010). Dominant and diet-responsive groups of bacteria within the human colonic microbiota. The ISME Journal, 5(2), pp.220-230.
49. Daly, K., Darby, A. and Shirazi-Beechey, S. (2016). Low calorie sweeteners and gut microbiota. Physiology & Behavior, 164, pp.494-500.
50. Suez, J., Korem, T., Zeevi, D., Zilberman-Schapira, G., Thaiss, C., Maza, O., Israeli, D., Zmora, N., Gilad, S.,Weinberger, A., Kuperman, Y., Harmelin, A., Kolodkin-Gal, I., Shapiro, H., Halpern, Z., Segal, E. and Elinav, E. (2015). Artificial Sweeteners Induce Glucose Intolerance by Altering the Gut Microbiota. Obstetrical & Gynecological Survey, 70(1), pp.31-32.
51. Beards, E., Tuohy, K. and Gibson, G. (2010). A human volunteer study to assess the impact of confectionery sweeteners on the gut microbiota composition. British Journal of Nutrition, 104(05), pp.701-708.
52. Suez, J., Korem, T., Zilberman-Schapira, G., Segal, E. and Elinav, E. (2015). Non- caloric artificial sweeteners and the microbiome: findings and challenges. Gut Microbes, 6(2), pp.149-155.